Order For Similar Custom Papers & Assignment Help Services

Fill the order form details - writing instructions guides, and get your paper done.

Posted: June 1st, 2023

A Full Night’s Sleep at Home Improves Memory Performance in an Associative and Relational Learning Task
Nicolas Ribeiro, Yannick Gounden, and Veronique Quaglino
University of Picardy Jules Verne
This study evaluated the effect of sleep in mediating episodic memory performances
in a recognition task of visually associated and related elements. It also considered links
between memory performances, dreams mentation, and emotional salience of items. Two
groups of participants were studied; in the wake group, they stayed awake on a normal
day, and in the sleep group, they slept at home according to their usual sleep schedule and
context. Compared with the wake group, participants in the sleep group performed better
in the delayed associative and relational tests. Negative and positive emotional images
were better recognized by the participants than neutral ones with no group specificity.
Examination of dream reports suggested that reporting a dream related to the present
study was associated with an increase in recognition performances. Overall, our research
demonstrated how new associative and relational links can be consolidated in memory
following a full night’s sleep.
Keywords: sleep, memory, dreams, relational, associative
Memory is highly associative, and our everyday life experiences are integrated
into a dynamic memory network composed of an intertwinement of past event
traces and more recent ones (Nadel, Hupbach, Gomez, & Newman-Smith, 2012;
Versace et al., 2014). This study evaluated the effect of sleep in mediating episodic
memory performances in a recognition task of visually associated elements. More
specifically, we examined how visually associated images create new relationships in
memory during a learning process and the influence of these relations on
subsequent recognition performance after a full night’s sleep. In addition, we
considered the influence of emotional salience of items on memory. Finally, we
evaluated the effect of the incorporation of the studied material into dream
mentation on memory performance.
This article was published Online First April 16, 2020.
X Nicolas Ribeiro, Yannick Gounden, and Veronique Quaglino, Department of Psychology,
Research Center in Psychology: Cognition, Psychism and Organizations EA 7273, University of Picardy
Jules Verne.
We thank Cheyenne Delcourt who participated in this study as an undergraduate student.
We declare that there are no conflicts of interest for this research and that the study received no
financial support from any organization.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nicolas Ribeiro, Department of
Psychology, Research Center in Psychology: Cognition, Psychism and Organizations EA 7273, Université de Picardie Jules Verne, Chemin du Thil, 80000, Amiens, France. E-mail: [email protected]
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
171
Dreaming
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 30, No. 2, 171–188
ISSN: 1053-0797 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/drm0000130
Associative Processes in Memory
Associative memory refers to links that can be created in memory when
different perceptive elements are presented at the same time in our environment.
For instance, when two different images are presented at the same moment on a
screen. Relational memory refers to the links that exist in memory between
different elements that were not presented at the same time but share content
similarity. Such a relation can be based on two associations that have a similar
element in common. As an example of such associative and relational memories,
Alger and Payne (2016) have proposed that if a person knows how to get from
Place A to Place B and also from Place B to Place C, then he or she is also able to
get from Place A to Place C.
The scientific investigation of how associated items are memorized is a
long-standing transversal interest in psychology, and researchers have postulated
that associative mechanism plays, at least, a key role in human memory, if not to be
the core mechanism of memory itself (Anderson & Bower, 2014; Chalfonte &
Johnson, 1996; Fuster, 1997; Hebb, 1949; Hutchison, 2003; Thomson & Tulving,
1970; Wheeler & Treisman, 2002). This interest for associative links in memory can
be traced back to the emergence of experimental psychology (Baldwin, 1890; Davis,
2008). MacDougall (1904) specified how the presentation of one part of an
association allows memory to be reactivated for the other part. Baldwin (1890)
mentioned how images in the present, such as the face of a friend, recalls older
memories, including the context of the previous meeting with this friend. Interestingly, Baldwin (1890) also indicated how these associative phenomena can emerge
from the nature of the physiological support of cognition. He described it as “an
intricate network of fibrous and cellular tissue.” He also pointed out the way
information is related into the memory system by highlighting “the wide capacity it
affords for varied and related representations” (p. 193). Within the scope of this
study, relations refer to how associations that share similarities are related to
memory. Nadel et al. (2012) suggested that consolidation is not about an isolated
memory trace but is a dynamic process that leads to changes in the memory system.
They proposed that new experiences must be integrated in the memory system to
be remembered. For this integration, memory system needs to modify its previous
state. Thus, memory is about creating new links between newer and older
experiences by the means of mechanisms that reshape previously fixed memories.
These transformations affect memory system depending on similarities between
previous experiences and the ongoing experiences. As a simplification, this dynamic
process is essentially about associating and relating experiences.
Association Interference Task
Current considerations for associative learning and for the relational dimension deriving from these associations coincided with the creation of new research
paradigms. For instance, Preston, Shrager, Dudukovic, and Gabrieli (2004) devised
a paradigm, known as Associative Inference Tasks (AIT; Lau, Tucker, & Fishbein,
2010; Ragland et al., 2009). The rationales of AIT can be described as such: “If A
is associated with B and B is associated with C, then A is related to C.” In the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
172 RIBEIRO, GOUNDEN, AND QUAGLINO
present research, AB and BC are referred to as associations and CA as relations
(Armstrong, Kose, Williams, Woolard, & Heckers, 2012; Carpenter & Schacter,
2017; Ragland et al., 2009; Shohamy & Wagner, 2008; Tripathi & Mishra, 2017).
AIT evaluates the performance on recognizing associations of pair that were
previously presented and on recognizing the relational inference that were created
from these associations (Bunsey & Elchenbaum, 1996; Lau et al., 2010; Preston et
al., 2004). AIT has been initially used to investigate the neural structures influencing relational and associative learning (Preston & Eichenbaum, 2013). AIT was also
used to demonstrate the hippocampal contribution in mediating relational information in human participants (Preston et al., 2004). Preston and collaborators
(2004) used AIT with the aim of evaluating how individual experiences need to be
related or conjoined in new ways to deal with novel situations (Preston et al., 2004).
Later, Zeithamova, Dominick, and Preston (2012) used a modified version of AIT
to assess how hippocampus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation correlated during learning of associations. In the present study, we also chose to use the
AIT paradigm to investigate both associative and relational dimensions of memory.
Sleep, Dream, and Memory Learning
Investigations on associative and relational dimensions of memory have been
crossed with other factors, such as sleep. For instance, Lau et al. (2010) were
interested in how specific sleep stages of non-REM and REM sleep contributed to
memory learning. The authors suggested that sleep influences both associative and
relational learning. Interestingly, these dimensions have also been used to consider
the influence of additional factors (such as emotion) on sleep enhancement of
memory. For instance, Alger and Payne (2016) demonstrated that participants of a
nap group were less likely to forget association with emotionally neutral stimuli
than participants who stayed awake. Additionally to emotional valence, another
aspect of emotional salience, emotional arousal is complementary to evaluate how
sleep enhance memory (Wagner, Gais, & Born, 2001). These two dimensions of
emotional salience (valence and arousal) are considered in the present study, as
they are both relevant in their links with memory performance.
In the research proposed by Alger and Payne (2016), as in most studies
investigating the effect of sleep on associative inference, the sleep period consisted
of a daytime nap. Besides the fact that duration of sleep is likely to affect memory
consolidation process (Achermann & Borbély, 1999; Ambrosini & Giuditta, 2001;
Scullin & Gao, 2018), some researchers have also demonstrated that sleeping in a
laboratory setting is susceptible to affect sleep quality and dreams (Agnew Jr.,
Webb, & Williams, 1966; Baekeland & Hoy, 1971; Metersky & Castriotta, 1996;
Toussaint et al., 1997) An habituation phase is usually worthy (Le Bon et al., 2001).
For these reasons, in the present study we preferred a full night’s sleep occurring in
the usual home setting of participants. Although the assessment of overall sleep
duration and quality relies on subjective and behavioral assessments, which are less
precise than polysomnography, we suggest that a typical sleep context situation
could be informative and complementary to data obtained in a laboratory setting.
Dreaming is a prominent feature of sleep. Schredl (2017) has suggested how
dream content is related to memory performances. In search of similarities between
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SLEEP IMPROVES ASSOCIATIVE AND RELATIONAL MEMORY 173
dream and events encountered during waking period, Vallat, Chatard, Blagrove,
and Ruby (2018) evaluated how voice-recorded dream reports were linked with
waking life events of participants. They demonstrated that memories of life events
(encountered during wake) included in dreams were susceptible to changes over
times. For instance, the emotional tones tend to be attenuated toward a more
neutral valence when they are reported within dream reports (Vallat et al., 2018).
Wamsley and Stickgold (2019) replicated their own previous findings showing that
dreaming of a virtual maze navigation task can improve performance at this same
task. We propose in the present study to investigate the links between written
dream reports and associative and relational learning. For this purpose, besides the
memory task, we proposed to participants a computerized diary to collect written
dream reports and sleep information.
Problematic
The present study is conducted to investigate sleep benefit on associative and
relational memory performances when a usual sleep setting and schedule are
proposed. Our main expectation is to observe a quantitative benefit on memory
performance for participants who slept after learning compared with those who
remained awake. We also expected that participants indicating dreams about the
study would have better memory performances on both relational and associative
tasks. We also postulated that emotional salience would influence memory performance and that this influence would be different between participant who slept and
those who did not.
Method
Participants
The experimental sample consisted of 63 participants (age 21.03 3.47
years, gender 54 female, nine male). They were randomly assigned to the two
experimental groups: the sleep group (N 31) and the wake group (N 32).
Participants were recruited through online social media and advertisements posted
in the corridors of a French university. Students who participated in this study were
eligible for course credits. Inclusion criteria were absence of medically asserted
sleep disorder, of medically asserted psychiatric disorder, and of chronic medication
able to interfere with sleep and memory. The study was performed in accordance
with the ethical requirement of the French and European legislation. All participants were instructed about the procedure and completed an informed consent that
guaranteed confidentiality and anonymity of investigations. An identification
number corresponding to each participant was provided. Before experimentation,
for the purpose of pretesting, 42 other students were recruited among the graduate
students of French University (16 for the face data set pretest, 14 for the
photography data set pretest, and 12 for the full protocol testing).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
174 RIBEIRO, GOUNDEN, AND QUAGLINO
Materials
The memory task (material and procedure steps) is based on the work of Alger
and Payne (2016) who used an AIT. As a reminder, the rationales of AIT can be
described as such: “If A is associated with B and B is associated with C, then A is
related to C” (see Figure 1).
Associations material. There were 80 visual associations between color
photographs of faces (A and C) in a light gray background and pictures of objects
(like manufactured objects, scenes, animals, etc.) presented in two blocks of 40
associations. Using the labeling used before, color photographs of faces were
Elements A and C and pictures of objects were Elements B.
All the photographs of faces were different (A and C), whereas the pictures of
objects (B) were identical from one block to another. Across the various associations, the position of the photographs of faces (A or C) and the pictures of objects
(B) were randomized (left or right) during the learning phase. All pictures of
objects (B) were displayed with 350px width and 275px height. All photographs of
faces were displayed with 220px width and 275px height.
The faces (A and B) consisted of 80 different photographs (front-faced) from
the FEI database (Thomaz & Giraldi, 2010). There were 40 female faces and 40
male faces. A pretest on 10 participants served to the selection of the faces with the
most neutral expression from this database (a slider ranging from negative, neutral,
and positive was proposed).
The pictures of objects (B) consisted of 40 images (14 negatives, 14 positives,
and 12 neutral) from the OASIS database (Kurdi, Lozano, & Banaji, 2017). The
OASIS database offers 822 images divided into several themes such as manufactured and animate objects and scenes. We proposed images with different
emotional salience. A pretest (N 14) was conducted to ensure that the content of
the image was well understood and that the emotional valence evoked by the
picture of the object was the most positive, neutral, or negative possible (Alger &
Payne, 2016). Emotional valence was evaluated using the Self-Assessment Manikin
Figure 1. The AIT was composed of a learning phase (AB and BC), an immediate associative
recognition phase (AB and BC), and two types of delayed recognition. AIT Associative Inference
Tasks.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SLEEP IMPROVES ASSOCIATIVE AND RELATIONAL MEMORY 175
(Bradley & Lang, 1994). The Self-Assessment Manikin can be used to assess
emotional valence or arousal on a 9-point scale. On this scale are five highly stylized
drawings of a person (the manikin) expressing different dimensions of emotion. For
valence, emotion ranged from very positive to very negative with the center
corresponding to neutral emotion.
Finally, to avoid any confusion between the pictures of objects, we selected
only one image per theme proposed in the OASIS database. In addition, to avoid
confusion between photographs of faces and pictures of objects, we excluded
pictures of objects containing faces facing the camera.
Four different lists of associations were created and counterbalanced among
participants. Associations in Lists 1 and 2 were identical, but the Blocks 1 and 2 were
inverted. In other words, the first block of 40 associations in the List 1 was identical to
the second block in the List 2. In the same way, associations presented in Lists 3 and
4 were identical but Blocks 1 and 2 were inverted. Objects (B) that were paired with
a female face (A or C) in the Lists 1 and 2 were paired with a male face (A or C) in
the Lists 3 and 4. Presentation order within each block was fully randomized.
Tasks procedure. The experimental procedure consisted of a learning phase, an
immediate associative recognition phase, a delayed relational recognition phase, and a
delayed associative phase (see Figure 1 below). The whole AIT was computerized and
elaborated with HTML5, JavaScript, and PHP languages. The software was accessible
within any up-to-date web browser and compatible with smartphone devices.
During learning phases, the instructions indicated to the participant that he or
she will see pairs of photographs composed of a face and an image for 5 s and that
he or she is requested to memorize these pairs and recognize them later. For each
pair, they were encouraged to imagine a link between the two photographs as
proposed by Alger and Payne (2016). The first 40 associations (AB) were displayed
on the screen for 5 s each and were followed by a white fixation cross displayed on
a black background for 1 s. The 40 following associations (BC) were displayed on
the screen for 5 s each and were followed by a white fixation cross displayed on a
black background for 1 s.
During the immediate associative recognition phase (AB/BC), each view
consisted of a face presented with four objects. The participant had to identify the
object (B) presented with the face (A or C) before. The distractors were already
seen objects that were paired with same-gender photographs of a face. Recognition
order was fully randomized inside each block. After each recognition, we proposed
an arousal question using the self-assessment manikin regarding the mental
association they elaborated. The wording for the 9-point scale used to rate arousal
was as follows (translated from French):
Irrespective of whether it is negative, positive or neutral, an emotion can provoke reactions
with different intensities. It can be calming or exciting. The left side of the scale below is used
to indicate an emotion that calms, soothes, relaxes. . . . The right side for an emotion that
activates or excites. . . . The center for an emotion that neither soothe or excite.
Questions were presented in a white font on a black background. The procedure
was identical for both the first block (AB) and the second block (BC) of 40
associations each.
The delayed relational recognition phase was proposed after 12-hr delay. For
each of the 40 relational recognition, a main photograph of a face (AC) was
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
176 RIBEIRO, GOUNDEN, AND QUAGLINO
presented alongside four other previously seen photographs of a face. One was
related to the main photograph and the other three distractors were related to other
faces across the learning phases. A question asked if the content of their dream was
linked to their answer.
For the delayed associative recognition phase (AB/BC), the task was identical
to the immediate associative recognition phase. In addition, participants were also
asked if the content of their dream was linked to their answer.
Sleep diary. The instruction of the diary indicated that as soon as they wake
up, the participants should use the diary on their smartphones to describe, as fully
as possible, all their dreams, impressions, and thoughts during the past night. They
were advised to put a text reminder on their alarm clock. All day long, if they
recalled a dream or a night, though, they were requested to add or update their
diary entry and indicate the time of the day.
The smartphone diary was developed to be ready to launch in seconds after
participants awakened. It was developed in dark shade to limit emission of light
(especially blue light), and all questions were displayed on the same page. A
textbook permitted the reporting of dream contents. List questions asked the
participant to indicate: the delay since awakening (in minutes), the sleep type
(night’s sleep or nap), subjective sleep quality ranging from 0 (good) to 4 (not good
at all), the sleep duration ranging from 10 min to 12 hr or more, and the links
between dream mentation and the current research. Each participant also selected
from a list, the response that corresponded to his or her situation: whether he or she
thought that the research did or not influenced the content of his or her dream. The
software automatically generated a timestamp when the diary was open.
The diary was identical for both groups. It was therefore necessary to provide
a task to which both groups could refer to answer questions about the study’s
influence on the content of their dream. For the sleep group, the instruction for this
task was to repeat internally for 5 min before going to bed this sentence “in my
dreams, I will see the things that I saw in this study.” For the wake group, as
participants had not seen the associations yet, they were instructed to repeat
internally for 5 min before going to bed the following sentence “in my dreams, I will
see the things that I ate today.”
To identify which items were incorporated in the dream report, four independent readers, blind to the objectives and assumptions of the present study, were
asked to identify which specific items the dream reports were related. The threshold
for considering that an item was incorporated into the dream content was set at the
majority of the readers (three or more designations out of four).
Questionnaire. We also used two questionnaires in this study. In the initial
questionnaire, we gathered demographic data (gender and age), dream recall
frequency (number of mornings that dreams were recalled during the last 21 days)
of participants, the average duration needed to fall asleep, and their usual sleep
duration. In the final questionnaire using a yes–no response, participants were
asked whether they were able or not to influence their dream according to the
instruction they received.
Dream evaluation. To summarize, dreams were assessed at three different
points in this study. In the Sleep Diary, a drop-down list labeled “Relationship to
current study” had two proposals. The first was “I don’t think the current study
influenced the content of this dream report” and the second was “I think the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
SLEEP IMPROVES ASSOCIATIVE AND RELATIONAL MEMORY 177
current study influenced this dream report.” During the associative recognition,
there was a “Yes, I don’t know, or No,” question with the following wording: “In
your opinion, was the content of your dreams related to (or influenced) this
answer.” In the final questionnaire, the wording of the yes or no question was:
“Were you able to influence your dreams in the way indicated by the instructions?”
Study Design
Participants were invited to visit a web page that randomly assigned them to
one of the two groups (sleep or wake). The study lasted 3 days (Thursday, Friday,
and Saturday). A detailed description of the procedure is available in Figure 2.
The first day, all participants had to connect to the web page to acknowledge
a step-by-step procedure and fill-in the initial questionnaire. They were also called
for a telephone interview that made it possible for them to fully understand the
protocol. During this interview, we responded to any question they had about the
study and informed them of the opportunity to contact the researcher if they had
any further questions. They were also guided to install the diary on their
smartphone. For the first morning after the learning phase, participants were asked
to fill in their dream diary as soon as they woke up. They also had the opportunity
to complete it at will during the day. If participants reported dreams during the
night, the reports were automatically added to their morning reports. As in Alger
and Payne (2016), participants were only aware of the immediate and delayed
associative testing.
To start the learning phase, it was necessary to provide a valid identification
number, and as a protection against uncontrolled practice during the protocol, the
software recorded every launch and sent the data automatically to a secured server.
After a 12-hr delay during which they slept or stayed awake (depending on their
group), participants had to perform the recognition phases (see Figure 1).
The last day, they had to complete the final questionnaire. After completing
the questionnaire, they were automatically notified that the study was finished.
Figure 2. Procedure during the 3-day protocol for the sleep and wake groups.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
178 RIBEIRO, GOUNDEN, AND QUAGLINO
Results
All statistics in the following sections were performed using the R software
(Ihaka & Gentleman, 1996). For the initial questionnaire, comparisons were
performed on sleep quality and demographic data. No significant difference was
found between the two groups. The significance level was set at p .05 for all
comparisons.
Memory Performances
The memory performance scores were based on the number of correct
recognition for the immediate associative recognition phase (on 80), the delayed
associative recognition phase (on 80), and the delayed relational recognition phase
(on 40; Table 1).
For associative recognition, a repeated measure analysis of variance
(ANOVA) was performed (see Figure 3). Condition (sleep or wake) served as
between-subjects factor and type of recognition (immediate/delayed) served as
within factors. There was a significant effect on the type of recognition, F(2, 61)
98.99, p .001, and an interaction effect with testing periods (immediate or
delayed) of recognition and group, F(2, 61) 6.26, p .015. To be specific, the
sleep group score (M 65.4, SD 8.79) was not significantly different to the wake
group score (M 63.7, SD 10.1) during the immediate recall. However, the sleep
group score (M 58.1, SD 9.99) was significantly greater than the wake group
score (M 51.5, SD 12.7) during the delayed recall.
To take account of individual variations in immediate performances, a delta
score was calculated following the strategy used by Alger and Payne (2016). The
wake group (M 12.19, SD 9.18) was compared with the sleep group (M
7.29, SD 5.64). Result of the t test confirmed the hypothesis of a less important
deterioration of performance for the sleep group (p .007, Cohen’s d .63).
For each immediate and delayed recognition of a given association, four
different profiles are possible along the learning process: Incorrect–Incorrect,
Incorrect–Correct, Correct–Incorrect, Correct–Correct. We examine whether these
four profiles are represented differently in the two groups, as they refer to distinct
memory fates. For example, the correct–incorrect profile refers to associations that
are known but forgotten during the learning process and the correct– correct profile
refers to associations that are correctly learned and sufficiently consolidated during
the learning process. In other words, each fate can reasonably be considered as
Table 1
Mean (M) and Standard Deviation (SD) for the Recognition Score on the Three Phases of AIT
Phases
Sleep group
M (SD)
Wake group
M (SD)

  • Immediate associative score/80 65.41 (8.80) 63.69 (10.07)
  • Delayed associative score/80 58.13 (9.99) 51.50 (12.74)
  • Delayed relational score/40 20.39 (5.56) 17 (5.80)
    Note. AIT Associative Inference Tasks.
    This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
    This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    SLEEP IMPROVES ASSOCIATIVE AND RELATIONAL MEMORY 179
    referring to distinct types of consolidation that occurred during the learning
    process. The results of both groups (sleep or wake) are presented in Table 2.
    A delayed relational score was based on the number of correct recognition
    during the delayed relational recognition phase. To test our oriented hypothesis of
    a sleep benefit on relational memory performance, a one-tailed student’s t test was
    performed between the sleep (M 20.4, SD 5.56) and the wake group (M 17,
    SD 5.80) on this score. The test confirmed the hypothesis of an enhancement for
    the recognition of relational pairs (p .011, Cohen’s d .60). Concerning the
    relational recognition performance, we performed analysis of covariance controlling for the performance at the immediate associative recognition phase. There was
    a significant effect of the group (sleep and wake) on the relational performance
    after controlling for performance at the immediate associative recognition phase,
    F(2, 61) 5.82, p .019.
    Figure 3. Comparison of memory performance at immediate and delayed associative recognition
    phases.
    Table 2
    Independent Comparisons of the Four Recognition Profiles for Immediate and Delayed Associative
    Phases
    Immediate
    recognition
    Delayed
    recognition
    Sleep group
    M (SD)
    Wake group
    M (SD) p value Cohen’s d
    Incorrect Incorrect 8.00 (6.95) 10.09 (7.83) .282 0.49
    Incorrect Correct 6.58 (3.31) 6.22 (4.10) .702 0.09
    Correct Incorrect 13.87 (4.49) 18.41 (7.59) .015 0.73
    Correct Correct 51.55 (11.27) 45.28 (13.78) .059 0.50
    Note. Nonparametric tests were used in case of violation of the normality assumptions.
    This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
    This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    180 RIBEIRO, GOUNDEN, AND QUAGLINO
    Effect of Emotional Valence and Arousal
    Memory performance was different depending on the emotional valence, F(1,
    61) 75.71, p .001, and not significant for the interaction between valence and
    group, F(1, 61) .569, p .45. A pairwise t test post hoc analysis comparing each
    valence revealed that performance for the neutral valence (M 35.7, SD 6.61)
    was significantly less recognized than negative (M 42.1, SD 7.70) and positive
    (M 41.5, SD 6.99) valence (p .001), whereas positive valence was not
    significantly different from negative emotional valence.
    For relational recognition, a repeated measure ANOVA was performed
    with emotional valence as within-subject factor (negative, neutral, and positive),
    the group as between-subjects factor, and the associative performance of the
    immediate associations as covariance. The results were significantly different for
    emotional valence, F(1, 61) 4.26, p .016, but no interaction effects were
    demonstrated, F(1, 61) 0.79, p .51. Specifically, post hoc comparisons
    revealed that neutral valence (M 5.99, SD 2.54) was less recognized than
    negative (M 7.23, SD 2.22, p .013) and positive (M 7.23, SD 2.53,
    p .002) valence, whereas negative and positive valence were not significantly
    different.
    For the arousal reported by the participant during recognition, the period
    of recognition (immediate/delayed) and the correctness of recognition (association correctly recognized or not) served as an intrasubject factor and the group
    (sleep/wake) as a within-subject factor. Comparisons were significant for
    recognition period, F(1, 61) 8.89, p .004, and correctness, F(1, 61) 97.58,
    p .001. No significant interaction was observed between correctness and
    recognition period. These results suggest that correct recognition at immediate
    (M 3.86, SD 0.11) and delayed recall (M 3.81, SD 0.19) had a higher
    arousal score than incorrect recognition at immediate (M 3.59, SD 0.34)
    and delayed recall (M 3.55, SD 0.35).
    Dream and Memory
    Dream diary. As a reminder, the question in the diary evaluated the link with
    the whole experimental situation and memory performances. Answers were related to
    very different aspects of the study such as participating in a study “I dreamed that I was
    being forced to do an experiment against my will.” Or the associations seen during the
    learning such as “The girl who is tanning in the pictures seen in the experiment.”
    Analysis of the diary revealed that there were nine out of 32 participants in the wake
    group to report one dream or more about the study and 18 out of 31 participants in the
    sleep group to report one dream or more about the study. These 18 participants did not
    perform significantly better than the others in the study. The descriptive results are
    summarized in the Table 3.
    To identify and address the reports in which dreams were linked to specific
    items of the study, four independent evaluators, blind to the objectives and
    assumptions of our research, were recruited. They were requested to identify the
    specific items to which the dream reports were linked. For the sleep group, this
    This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
    This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    SLEEP IMPROVES ASSOCIATIVE AND RELATIONAL MEMORY 181
    revealed that the dreams of 10 out of 18 participants could be linked to one or more
    specific elements, with the remaining eight participants reporting dreams that were
    related only to participation in a study.
    For these 10 participants, 14 different items were identified. These items were
    logically involved in 28 associations (each item was involved in two associations
    according to the properties of the AIT). Among them, 26 were correctly recognized
    by the participants. All items identified by the independent readers were correctly
    recognized in the delayed recognition.
    As indicated in the paragraph on delayed recognition, participants were asked
    to indicate if they believed they dreamed of the association element. Ten out of the
    14 items were reported by participants as dreamed. All associations among these 26
    elements which were reported as dreamed were correctly identified.
    Delayed recognition. Participants in the wake group did not sleep between
    the learning phase and the recognition phase, whereas participants in the sleep
    group did sleep between learning and recognition. During the delayed recognition
    of associations, 15 out of 31 participants in the sleep group reported dreaming about
    one or more associations and eight out of 32 participants in the wake group
    reported dreaming about one or more associations in the past. A comparison of the
    number of associations pointed out by the participants as related to dreams
    between the Sleep group (M 2.81, SD 5.14) and the Wake group (M 0.91,
    SD 2.40) was performed using a Mann–Whitney test. It suggested a significant
    difference between these groups (U 362, p .033).
    As a reminder, participants indicated if they had dreamed about the association by choosing among these responses: “Yes, I don’t know, No.” A one-way
    ANOVA that compared all delayed associative scores depending on the response
    of the participants was performed, F(2, 283) 35.2, p .001. Tuckey’s post hoc
    analysis revealed that associations indicated as dreamed (a Yes response; M 0.91,
    SD 0.29) were better recognized compared with those that were supposedly not
    dreamed (No response; M 0.69, SD 0.47) or for which the participant did not
    know if he or she had dreamed about them (M 0.63, SD 0.48).
    Final questionnaire. In the final questionnaire, 16 out of 30 participants
    reported dreaming about the study in the sleep group. We performed a student’s t
    test comparing the task performance of those who reported dreams about the study
    and those who did not. Only the comparison of memory performance on delayed
    recognition of the associations was significant (p .016, Cohen’s d .91). To be
    descriptive, participants in the sleep group who reported dreaming about the study
    Table 3
    Scores for the Wake Group and Sleep Group Participants Who Reported Dreaming About the Study in
    the Final Questionnaire
    Phases
    Wake group Sleep group
    Dream
    (n 9)
    No dream
    (n 23)
    Dream
    (n 18)
    No dream
    (n 13)
    Immediate recognition 61.9 (10.65) 64.4 (9.99) 67.1 (5.07) 63.1 (12.11)
    Delayed recognition 47.6 (11.4) 53 (13.14) 60.3 (5.98) 55.2 (13.50)
    Relational performances 15 (5.74) 17.8 (5.75) 20.9 (4.09) 19.6 (7.24)
    Note. All scores are reported as such: M (SD).
    This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
    This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    182 RIBEIRO, GOUNDEN, AND QUAGLINO
    in the final questionnaire had an immediate associative score (M 70, SD 5.39)
    that did not differ from those who did not report any dream about the study (M
    66.50, SD 10.67). Participants in the sleep group who reported dreaming about
    the study in the final questionnaire had a better delayed associative score (M
    62.50, SD 5.62) than participants who did not report any dream about the study
    (M 54.10, SD 11.56). Participants in the sleep group who reported dreaming
    about the study in the final questionnaire did not obtain a significantly different
    delayed relational score (M 22.10, SD 1.33) than those who did not report any
    dream about the study (M 18.80, SD 1.40). In other words, participants of the
    sleep group who retrospectively indicated dreaming about the study had better
    performances in associative recognition that those who did not.
    In the wake group, they were four out of 31 participants to report dreaming
    about the study in the final questionnaire. We performed a student’s t test
    comparing the task performances of participants in the wake group who reported
    dreams about the study and those who did not. No comparison for the wake group
    was significant. Overall, participants in the wake group who reported dreaming
    about the study in the final questionnaire had an immediate associative score (M
    60.8, SD 4.76) that did not differ from those who did not report any dream about
    the study (M 64.2, SD 10.74). Participants in the wake group who reported
    dreaming about the study in the final questionnaire did not obtain any significantly
    different delayed associative score (M 51.6, SD 14.89) than those who did not
    report any dream in relation to the study (M 51.5, SD 12.63).
    Complementary Analysis
    For the sleep group, the average sleep duration was 7.21 hr (SD 1.55) in the
    diary and 7.47 (SD 0.99) in the initial questionnaire. For the wake group, the
    average sleep duration was 7.55 (SD 1.85) when reported in the diary and 7.31
    (SD 1.33) in the initial questionnaire. The student’s t test revealed no significant
    effect, t(61) 0.6, p .55.
    The average delay between the learning phase and the delayed recognition
    phase was 12.14 hr (SD 0.53) in the sleep group and 12.17 hr (SD 0.41) in the
    wake group. The t test revealed no significant effect, t(61) 0.25, p .80.
    To summarize, none of the comparisons we performed on these duration were
    significant, suggesting no difference between usual sleep schedules and sleep
    schedules in the present study. There was also no significant difference between the
    two groups on these variables.
    Assignment help – Discussion
    The present study was conducted to investigate how a full night’s sleep in a
    home setting could influence the recognition of visual associations and relational
    inferences based on these associations. Our expectations were to observe a
    quantitative benefit on memory consolidation for participants who slept after
    learning compared with those who remained awake. We also hypothesized that
    emotional salience would influence memory performances. Finally, the link beThis document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    SLEEP IMPROVES ASSOCIATIVE AND RELATIONAL MEMORY 183
    tween dreaming and memory performances has been evaluated through retrospective evaluation questionnaires and a diary.
    Sleep Impact on Associative and Relational Processes in Memory
    Sleep has an important role in memory consolidation (Stickgold, 2005). In this
    study, we investigated this role and focalized our investigations on the associative
    dimension of memory and on the relations that emerged between these associations. We used the AIT, as it was specifically elaborated to allow these two
    dimensions (associative and relational) and to demonstrate memory enhancement
    for both (Bunsey & Elchenbaum, 1996). As expected in the present study,
    participants who slept had better performances for both associative and relational
    learning than participants who remained awake (Alger & Payne, 2016). Such
    enhancement was previously studied using laboratory investigations and different
    methodological designs. For instance, Ellenbogen and collaborators (2007) tested
    the effect of sleep on associative and relational memory. With a delay between
    learning and recognition similar to the present study, they showed a benefit of sleep
    and named this benefit an inferential boost. Such benefit was also demonstrated
    with nap design in the study of Lau et al. (2010) and of Alger and Payne (2016). Our
    study differed from previous research, as we investigated the sleep benefit for
    associative and relational memory performance using AIT, in the context of a usual
    sleep schedule in a home setting with a 12-hr delay. The fact that memory
    enhancement occurred in such varying situations is a robust confirmation of how
    sleep is critical for memory, notably on the maintenance of its associative and
    relational properties (Diekelmann, Wilhelm, Wagner, & Born, 2013; Klinzing,
    Rasch, Born, & Diekelmann, 2016).
    When a memory is activated during wake times, it tends to lead to changes in
    the memory trace. In contrast, it is interesting to note that this activation seems to
    lead to stabilization during sleep, making it more resistant to interference and
    therefore more likely to be recalled as in the present study (Diekelmann, Büchel,
    Born, & Rasch, 2011; Klinzing et al., 2016). The effect of sleep on associative
    recognition performances operated mainly on correctly learned associations in the
    present study. This corroborated previous results that assumed that sleep has a
    protective effect on previously well-integrated associations (Alger & Payne, 2016;
    Schreiner & Rasch, 2018).
    Emotion Salience, Memory, Dream, and Sleep
    The fact that emotions have a beneficial effect on memory has been extensively
    demonstrated in the past (Reisberg & Hertel, 2003). In our study, emotional
    salience (valence and arousal) is, as expected, linked to better memory performances for the associative and relational dimensions. However, there was no
    significant interaction between sleep, emotion, and memory. With short periods of
    sleep occurring in the afternoon, Alger and Payne (2016) showed an advantage of
    sleep period (comparatively to wake period) over the most emotionally neutral
    associations. In addition, Sopp, Michael, and Mecklinger (2018) studied the
    This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
    This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    184 RIBEIRO, GOUNDEN, AND QUAGLINO
    enhancing effect of a morning nap on associative memory by considering the
    emotional salience of their objects. Like in the present study, their main analysis
    showed a group, time, and emotional salience effect, as well as an interaction effect
    between time and group. Interestingly, they also studied the nature of emotional
    salience effect on associative memory and showed that when the background
    scene of their associated pair (object and background) was negative and highly
    arousing, it had a detrimental effect on memory performances. It is noticeable
    that such detrimental effect did not appear in our present results based on a full
    night’s sleep.
    Among possible explanations, and as a prospect for future studies, it is possible
    that REM-rich sleep and NREM sleep have specific benefits on emotions and that
    their selective effect was combined in this study (Alger & Payne, 2016; Sopp et al.,
    2018). Another interesting point of discussion is that in this research, we encouraged participants to imagine a link between the two photographs without controlling the meaning of this link for them (Alger & Payne, 2016). It is thus difficult to
    establish how the current protocol induced authentic emotions or simply depicted
    emotions without any implication for oneself. As an argument for greater control
    in future studies, Conway argued that emotional content will affect episodic
    memory depending on how the self is at stake (Conway, 2005, 2009). Bennion,
    Payne, and Kensinger (2016) also discussed how information relevant to the future
    will influence the interaction between sleep, emotions, and memory. Therefore, we
    recommend that future studies should provide a situation that truly evokes relevant
    emotions for the participant rather than simply suggesting it with the material.
    A salient feature of sleep is the presence of dreams, which have also been
    posited to influence memory performance (Schredl, 2017). Consistent with recent
    research, the results of the present study demonstrated a link between dream
    content and memory performance (Schoch, Cordi, Schredl, & Rasch, 2019;
    Wamsley & Stickgold, 2019). In the present study, participants who indicated
    retrospectively to have dreamed about the associations had better performances
    than those who did not. This is in line with the research of Ribeiro, Gounden, and
    Quaglino (2016) illustrating how evaluation of sleep and dream contents by the
    means of a smartphone dream diary is informative. Diary evaluation is also
    complementary to a range of retrospective evaluations of sleep and dream activities
    (Ribeiro, Gounden, & Quaglino, 2018). Schredl and collaborators (2019) have
    shown that written report, as in the present study, is susceptible to gather more
    dream report and links with real life. However, reports with this methodology are
    generally shorter than other methods such as voice recorded reports. The number
    of dreams that one can report with only one wake per night can only underestimate
    the actual number of dreams he or she experiences during the whole night (Schoch
    et al., 2019). Consequently, our result, in which we were able to identify links
    between dream reports and memory performances, is encouraging. They indicate
    that the link between content of conscious experience during sleep and episodic
    experience during the day exists and is robust enough to be perceptible with a diary
    methodology.
    The fact that we explicitly encouraged our participants to voluntarily influence
    their dreams may have led to a possible compliance phenomenon that limits the
    scope of our observations. However, it is to be noted that the wake group did not
    elicit much inclusion in their dreams while they were encouraged to influence them.
    This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
    This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    SLEEP IMPROVES ASSOCIATIVE AND RELATIONAL MEMORY 185
    We nevertheless suggest that future studies should avoid such dream-induction
    solicitations to fully demonstrate that dream enhancement on associative and
    relational links occurs naturally.
    Conclusion and Perspective
    In conclusion, this study is a demonstration that a full night’s sleep at home
    improves memory, both at the associative and relational dimensions. Additionally,
    emotionally salient associations were better recognized without a significant
    influence of sleep. We proposed that future studies should evoke emotion rather
    than simply suggesting it with the material. Finally, our results also showed that
    dream experiences are beneficial for memory performance. As a future step, it
    could be interesting to influence the content of dreams to evaluate any subsequent
    improvements in associative and relational performances.
    References
    Achermann, P., & Borbély, A. A. (1999). Sleep homoeostasis and models of sleep regulation. In M. H.
    Kryger, T. Roth, & W. C. Dement (Eds.), Principles and practice of sleep medicine (5th ed., pp.
    431–444). St. Louis, MO: Elsevier. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-1-4160-6645-3.00037-2
    Agnew, H. W., Jr., Webb, W. B., & Williams, R. L. (1966). The first night effect: An EEG study of sleep.
    Psychophysiology, 2, 263–266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1966.tb02650.x
    Alger, S. E., & Payne, J. D. (2016). The differential effects of emotional salience on direct associative
    and relational memory during a nap. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 16,
    1150 –1163. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0460-1
    Ambrosini, M. V., & Giuditta, A. (2001). Learning and sleep: The sequential hypothesis. Sleep Medicine
    Reviews, 5, 477–490. http://dx.doi.org/10.1053/smrv.2001.0180
    Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (2014). Human associative memory. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315802886
    Armstrong, K., Kose, S., Williams, L., Woolard, A., & Heckers, S. (2012). Impaired associative inference
    in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 622–629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/
    sbq145
    Baekeland, F., & Hoy, P. (1971). Reported vs recorded sleep characteristics. Archives of General
    Psychiatry, 24, 548 –551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1971.01750120064011
    Baldwin, J. M. (1890). Division of the intellectual functions. In J. M. Baldwin (Ed.), Handbook of
    psychology: Senses and intellect (Rev. 2nd ed., pp. 80 –81). New York, NY: Henry Holt and
    Company. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10927-006
    Bennion, K. A., Payne, J. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2016). The impact of napping on memory for
    future-relevant stimuli: Prioritization among multiple salience cues. Behavioral Neuroscience, 130,
    281–289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bne0000142
    Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion – The self-assessment manikin and the
    semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49 –59. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9
    Bunsey, M., & Elchenbaum, H. (1996). Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and
    humans. Nature, 379, 255–257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/379255a0
    Carpenter, A. C., & Schacter, D. L. (2017). Flexible retrieval: When true inferences produce false
    memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 335–349.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000340
    Chalfonte, B. L., & Johnson, M. K. (1996). Feature memory and binding in young and older adults.
    Memory and Cognition, 24, 403–416. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200930
    Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 594 –628. http://dx
    .doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005
    Conway, M. A. (2009). Episodic memories. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2305–2313. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
    j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.003
    Davis, S. F. (2008). Handbook of research methods in experimental psychology. Oxford, United
    Kingdom: Wiley.
    This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
    This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    186 RIBEIRO, GOUNDEN, AND QUAGLINO
    Diekelmann, S., Büchel, C., Born, J., & Rasch, B. (2011). Labile or stable: Opposing consequences for
    memory when reactivated during waking and sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 381–386. http://dx.doi
    .org/10.1038/nn.2744
    Diekelmann, S., Wilhelm, I., Wagner, U., & Born, J. (2013). Sleep improves prospective remembering
    by facilitating spontaneous-associative retrieval processes. PLoS ONE, 8, e77621. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1371/journal.pone.0077621
    Ellenbogen, J. M., Hu, P. T., Payne, J. D., Titone, D., & Walker, M. P. (2007). Human relational memory
    requires time and sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
    America, 104, 7723–7728. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0700094104
    Fuster, J. M. (1997). Network memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 451–459. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
    S0166-2236(97)01128-4
    Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York, NY: Wiley.
    Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A
    microanalytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10, 785–813. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/
    BF03196544
    Ihaka, R., & Gentleman, R. (1996). R: A language for data analysis and graphics. Journal of
    Computational and Graphical Statistics, 5, 299 –314.
    Klinzing, J. G., Rasch, B., Born, J., & Diekelmann, S. (2016). Sleep’s role in the reconsolidation of
    declarative memories. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 136, 166 –173. http://dx.doi.org/10
    .1016/j.nlm.2016.10.004
    Kurdi, B., Lozano, S., & Banaji, M. R. (2017). Introducing the open affective standardized image set
    (OASIS). Behavior Research Methods, 49, 457–470. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0715-3
    Lau, H., Tucker, M. A., & Fishbein, W. (2010). Daytime napping: Effects on human direct associative
    and relational memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 93, 554 –560. http://dx.doi.org/10
    .1016/j.nlm.2010.02.003
    Le Bon, O., Staner, L., Hoffmann, G., Dramaix, M., San Sebastian, I., Murphy, J. R.,… Linkowski, P.
    (2001). The first-night effect may last more than one night. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 35,
    165–172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3956(01)00019-X
    MacDougall, R. (1904). Recognition and recall. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific
    Methods, 1, 229 –233. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2010991
    Metersky, M. L., & Castriotta, R. J. (1996). The effect of polysomnography on sleep position: Possible
    implications on the diagnosis of positional obstructive sleep apnea. Respiration, 63, 283–287.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000196561
    Nadel, L., Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., & Newman-Smith, K. (2012). Memory formation, consolidation and
    transformation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 1640 –1645. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
    j.neubiorev.2012.03.001
    Preston, A. R., & Eichenbaum, H. (2013). Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory.
    Current Biology, 23, R764 –R773. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.041
    Preston, A. R., Shrager, Y., Dudukovic, N. M., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Hippocampal contribution
    to the novel use of relational information in declarative memory. Hippocampus, 14, 148 –152.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20009
    Ragland, J. D., Cools, R., Frank, M., Pizzagalli, D. A., Preston, A., Ranganath, C., & Wagner, A. D.
    (2009). CNTRICS final task selection: Long-term memory. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35, 197–212.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn134
    Reisberg, D., & Hertel, P. (2003). Memory and emotion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    Ribeiro, N., Gounden, Y., & Quaglino, V. (2016). Investigating on the methodology effect when
    evaluating lucid dream. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1306. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01306
    Ribeiro, N., Gounden, Y., & Quaglino, V. (2018). Measure of dream recall frequency: Comparing
    retrospective and logbook evaluations. International Journal of Dream Research, 11, 66 –68.
    Schoch, S. F., Cordi, M. J., Schredl, M., & Rasch, B. (2019). The effect of dream report collection and
    dream incorporation on memory consolidation during sleep. Journal of Sleep Research, 28, e12754.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12754
    Schredl, M. (2017). Is dreaming related to sleep-dependent memory consolidation? In N. Axmacher &
    B. Rasch (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of memory consolidation. Studies in neuroscience, psychology and behavioral economics (pp. 173–182). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10
    .1007/978-3-319-45066-7_11
    Schredl, M., Dreer, J., Mösle, A., Rall, M., Rauch, L., Rose, S., & Seuffert, S. (2019). Voice-recorded vs.
    written dream reports: A research note. International Journal of Dream Research, 12, 138 –140.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2019.1.58801
    Schreiner, T., & Rasch, B. (2018). To gain or not to gain–The complex role of sleep for memory. Cortex,
    101, 282–287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.06.011
    Scullin, M. K., & Gao, C. (2018). Dynamic contributions of slow wave sleep and REM sleep to cognitive
    longevity. Current Sleep Medicine Reports, 4, 284 –293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40675-018-0131-6
    This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
    This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    SLEEP IMPROVES ASSOCIATIVE AND RELATIONAL MEMORY 187
    Shohamy, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2008). Integrating memories in the human brain: Hippocampalmidbrain encoding of overlapping events. Neuron, 60, 378 –389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron
    .2008.09.023
    Sopp, M. R., Michael, T., & Mecklinger, A. (2018). Effects of early morning nap sleep on associative
    memory for neutral and emotional stimuli. Brain Research, 1698, 29 –42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
    j.brainres.2018.06.020
    Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437, 1272–1278. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1038/nature04286
    Thomaz, C. E., & Giraldi, G. A. (2010). A new ranking method for principal components analysis and
    its application to face image analysis. Image and Vision Computing, 28, 902–913. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1016/j.imavis.2009.11.005
    Thomson, D. M., & Tulving, E. (1970). Associative encoding and retrieval: Weak and strong cues.
    Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 255–262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029997
    Toussaint, M., Luthringer, R., Schaltenbrand, N., Nicolas, A., Jacqmin, A., Carelli, G.,… Macher, J.-P.
    (1997). Changes in EEG power density during sleep laboratory adaptation. Sleep, 20, 1201–1207.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/sleep/20.12.1201
    Tripathi, R. P., & Mishra, G. R. (2017). Design and implementation of a real time stress monitoring system
    with the help of ECG using Matlab tool. 2017 International Conference on Computer, Communications and Electronics (Comptelix), IEEE (pp. 365–369).
    Vallat, R., Chatard, B., Blagrove, M., & Ruby, P. (2018). Characteristics of the memory sources of
    dreams: A new version of the content-matching paradigm to take mundane and remote memories
    into account. PLoS One, 13, e0193440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193440
    Versace, R., Vallet, G. T., Riou, B., Lesourd, M., Labeye, É., & Brunel, L. (2014). Act-in: An integrated
    view of memory mechanisms. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 280 –306. http://dx.doi.org/10
    .1080/20445911.2014.892113
    Wagner, U., Gais, S., & Born, J. (2001). Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals
    with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learning and Memory, 8, 112–119. http://dx.doi
    .org/10.1101/lm.36801
    Wamsley, E. J., & Stickgold, R. (2019). Dreaming of a learning task is associated with enhanced memory
    consolidation: Replication in an overnight sleep study. Journal of Sleep Research, 28, e12749.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12749
    Wheeler, M. E., & Treisman, A. M. (2002). Binding in short-term visual memory. Journal of
    Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 48 –64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.48
    Zeithamova, D., Dominick, A. L., & Preston, A. R. (2012). Hippocampal and ventral medial prefrontal
    activation during retrieval-mediated learning supports novel inference. Neuron, 75, 168 –179. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.010
    This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
    This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
    188 RIBEIRO, GOUNDEN, AND QUAGLINO

Order | Check Discount

Tags: Help write my thesis paper, I need help writing my dissertation thesis, I need help writing my PhD dissertation, I paid someone to write my dissertation, Masters dissertation help

Assignment Help For You!

Special Offer! Get 20-25% Off On your Order!

Why choose us

You Want Quality and That’s What We Deliver

Top Skilled Writers

To ensure professionalism, we carefully curate our team by handpicking highly skilled writers and editors, each possessing specialized knowledge in distinct subject areas and a strong background in academic writing. This selection process guarantees that our writers are well-equipped to write on a variety of topics with expertise. Whether it's help writing an essay in nursing, medical, healthcare, management, psychology, and other related subjects, we have the right expert for you. Our diverse team 24/7 ensures that we can meet the specific needs of students across the various learning instututions.

Affordable Prices

The Essay Bishops 'write my paper' online service strives to provide the best writers at the most competitive rates—student-friendly cost, ensuring affordability without compromising on quality. We understand the financial constraints students face and aim to offer exceptional value. Our pricing is both fair and reasonable to college/university students in comparison to other paper writing services in the academic market. This commitment to affordability sets us apart and makes our services accessible to a wider range of students.

100% Plagiarism-Free

Minimal Similarity Index Score on our content. Rest assured, you'll never receive a product with any traces of plagiarism, AI, GenAI, or ChatGPT, as our team is dedicated to ensuring the highest standards of originality. We rigorously scan each final draft before it's sent to you, guaranteeing originality and maintaining our commitment to delivering plagiarism-free content. Your satisfaction and trust are our top priorities.

How it works

When you decide to place an order with Dissertation App, here is what happens:

Complete the Order Form

You will complete our order form, filling in all of the fields and giving us as much detail as possible.

Assignment of Writer

We analyze your order and match it with a writer who has the unique qualifications to complete it, and he begins from scratch.

Order in Production and Delivered

You and your writer communicate directly during the process, and, once you receive the final draft, you either approve it or ask for revisions.

Giving us Feedback (and other options)

We want to know how your experience went. You can read other clients’ testimonials too. And among many options, you can choose a favorite writer.